Do your pets know when an earthquake is about to happen?
EarthSP? Why it may be nothing more than a few anecdotes.
Within minutes of Melbourne being rattled by yesterday’s earthquake, my Victorian friends reported changes in the behavior of their animals.
One friend wrote on social media that her dog Harvey stood in the hallway howling for five whole minutes before the earth moved. A colleague reported his television reception went fuzzy. Still, when he walked outside to check the aerial, he noted an “unusual and striking absence of birdsong” before he felt the quake.
My friend’s cat Henry inexplicably disappeared before the quake but returned home safe after a few hours. Conversely, her rough collie Angie — who is terrified of storms — was reported “totally chilled” before, during, and after the seismic event.
Earthquakes are unsettling, terrifying, and potentially fatal. Thirteen people lost their lives and 160 were injured in the 1989 Newcastle earthquake. If our animal companions can give us a heads-up when an event like this is about to happen, it could be truly lifesaving. But can they? Let’s have a look at the evidence.
The scholarly literature provides dozens of anecdotal reports of companion animals, livestock, wildlife, and even insects behaving strangely before earthquakes.
But a review of 180 publications reporting 700 records of abnormal or unusual animal behaviors before 160 earthquakes found the evidence correlating these behaviors with subsequent earthquakes was weak.
Most reports were anecdotal and were made after the earthquake, making them vulnerable to “recall bias.” Put, people may be more likely to interpret their animal’s behavior as strange in the light of a, particularly memorable or traumatic event.
To establish that unusual animal behaviors can predict earthquakes, scientists need to observe animals under controlled environmental conditions for extended periods — long enough to keep their behavior before, during, and after earthquakes. To be confident, animals do indeed behave strangely before an earthquake; we would also need to see them not behaving strangely when there isn’t an impending quake.
Sadly, the evidence doesn’t come close to satisfying this. But the authors of the review did find the supposed “predictive” behavior in animals occurred around the same time as “foreshocks” — more minor earthquakes that precede the main seismic event.
If this is the case, then what people interpret as animals’ ability to “predict” earthquakes may be reactions to the vibrations or sounds from earthquakes that are too faint for us humans to detect.
This wouldn’t be surprising, given that animals often outperform us regarding sensory perception, such as smell. And it makes sense, given almost 60 percent of unusual animal behaviors associated with earthquakes occurred in the five minutes preceding the quake.
Animal fright and flight
Fear, anxiety, or distress triggered by foreshocks might explain why animals display behaviors such as vocalizing (like Harvey, the howling dog) or fleeing to somewhere they feel safer (like Henry, the disappearing act).
But of course, it’s possible Harvey and Henry behaved like this for purely non-earthquake-related reasons, and the timing was sheer coincidence. There are many reasons a dog may howl (a courier opens the front gate) or a cat may go missing (your cat may hear a loud noise and hide under the bed), but we tend to make the connection only when we’re aware of the same stimuli.
What we do know is that animals can be seriously affected by earthquakes, whether through injury, displacement or compromised access to food and water. Thousands of animals and 185 people perished in the 2011 Christchurch earthquake, and many more animals were left homeless by property damage.
Yesterday’s quake is also an important reminder for people with companion animals to include them in emergency planning. Dogs and cats should be identified with a collar and be tagged and microchipped. And don’t forget to update the contact details if you move house or change your phone number — that way you’ll more easily be reunited.
This article was originally published on The Conversation by Anne Quain at the University of Sydney. Read the original article here.